Conspicuous cognition
Why I started this blog, why I called it "conspicuous cognition", and why the desire for social approval drives the evolutionary weirdness of our species.
This year, I am going to be blogging at this site. There are several reasons for this. I like writing. It helps me to clarify my thoughts about a topic. Blogs also provide a way of publicly engaging with ideas and debates that is separate from the long, soul-destroying process of academic publication. And I now have a permanent lectureship, which means I can express and argue for somewhat controversial takes without having to worry about losing all hope of an academic career.
The title of the blog is “Conspicuous Cognition.” On one level, it’s a cute play on Thorstein Veblen’s term “conspicuous consumption,” which refers to the consumption of goods and services not for practical reasons but to signal one’s status and wealth. On a deeper level, it reflects a central focus of my research: how the intense human desire for social approval shapes how people think about the world, both as individuals and as collectives.
In this first post, I will summarise what I mean by this, and then describe the kinds of things that I will be writing about here this year.
An ultra-social species
Most of human behaviour is guided by sensible, functional goals. We eat, we drink, we seek shelter, we piss and shit, we try to stay warm, we work, we have sex, we produce offspring, we care for our offspring, and so on. In these respects, we are much like other animals, and our goals make evolutionary sense.
However, much of what we do doesn’t seem to be guided by sensible, functional goals, and it doesn’t make much evolutionary sense. We produce elaborate art. We engage in strange, time-consuming rituals. We worship imaginary people and forces. We argue about abstract philosophical questions. We try to spead justice, fairness, and equality around the world. We investigate microbes and the comsos. We strive to be authentic. We write blog posts.
To understand such evolutionarily weird behaviours, we should embrace Veblen’s core insight about luxury goods and services: human behaviours that seem odd, irrational, and wasteful are often rooted in social goals.
Norms, gossip, and reputations
Humans are social animals. More precisely, we are ultra-social. We engage in, and depend on, an extreme amount of cooperation. In this respect we more closely resemble ultra-cooperative eusocial insects (e.g., ants and bees) than our great ape cousins. However, those insects cooperate with genetic relatives whose fitness interests are very closely aligned. In contrast, human cooperation occurs within large-scale, complex social networks featuring strategic, smart individuals with divergent fitness interests. Human cooperation is therefore much more interesting, complex, and challenging.
We achieve cooperation in many ways, but there are recurrent patterns that make up the background fabric of human social life. First, there are reputations. We constantly evaluate people’s competence (their ability to help others) and warmth (their willingness to help others). Those with good reputations win friends, partners, and social support; those with bad reputations lose them.
Second, there are social norms, community standards of thought and behaviour. Don’t lie, don’t attack group members, don’t be selfish, don’t covet your neighbour’s partner. Those who violate such norms are punished - sometimes just reputationally, sometimes in more direct ways.
Third, there is gossip: people obsessively share and seek out information about the actions, traits, mistakes, mishaps, and embarassments of others. If you mess up - if you’re selfish, hypocritical, exploitative, disgusting, and so on - other people will likely hear about it and adjust their feelings towards you.
Competition
At the same time, human beings are extremely competitive. Given our Darwinian origins, it could not be otherwise. People compete for resources, territory, mates, and friends. But like other social animals, we also compete for status.
In most social animals, status takes the form of dominance. Dominance hierarchies are rooted in fear and coercion. Our species also has dominance, but it sits uneasily with our reliance on cooperation. Given this, even those who benefit from the most extreme, clear-cut cases of domination - slavery, colonialism, rigid caste-based systems, and so on - try to obfuscate and frame the hierarchy in ways that save their cooperative reputation (“innate superiority”, “white man’s burden”, “the will of God”, etc.).
Dominance also sits uneasily with prestige, the other fundamental form of human social status. Prestige is rooted not in coercion but in admiration, respect, and voluntary deference. As Machievelli pointed out, social influence can be rooted in fear - but it can also be rooted in love. Taylor Swift is extremely prestigious in this sense.
People are crazy about prestige. To be admired, respected, deferred to - these are some of the most powerful human drives. At the same time, people only chase prestige because other people award it. From an evolutionary perspective, this seems odd and self-defeating. Dominance must be taken, but people seem to give prestige away for free. This is an illusion, however: deference, respect, and sychophancy are the price that people pay to access and encourage socially-valued traits, skills, information, and sacrifices (see here, here, and here).
The pursuit of prestige drives the most spectacular human achievements. People devote themselves to art, scientific advances, intellectual insights, and extraordinary acts of self-sacrifice and altruism. In doing so they are paid in the currency that people care most about: status.
Conspicuous cognition
Humans, then, are motivated to get along and get ahead - to win social approval and trust, and to compete for power and status. These drives explain much that is weird and wonderful in human behaviour, and they also help to illuminate the strangeness of human cognition.
Much of cognition - thought, deliberation, reasoning - is done for sensible, functional reasons. We build and revise broadly accurate mental models, and we share the contents of our mental models with other people through communication. Most of this is mundane, a matter of acquiring and sharing useful information with which to navigate the world to satisfy our desires. There is nothing very puzzling about it.
At the same time, much of human cognition is very different from this. Just as people consume goods and services not just for functional reasons but to show off and win social approval, much of human cognition - thought, deliberation, reasoning, argument, science, commentary, philosophy, public disagreement, substack posts, and so on - cannot be understood without understanding how it helps people to get along and get ahead in complex social games organised around norms, reputations, gossip, and status. For example:
our beliefs are shaped by social rewards and punishments and often function to signal our group allegiances.
People behave as instinctive propagandists for their favourite political and cultural tribes.
Intellectuals, pundits, and experts devote themselves to producing high-quality rationalisations of popular decisions and narratives in exchange for attention and status.
Human irrationality is often a team sport, scaffolded by social incentives that enable groups to promote and protected shared delusions.
People often act on self-interested motives but deny or obfuscate this fact to win social approval and trust. My forthcoming book “Why it’s OK to be cynical” will explore this phenomenon.
(These ideas are all greatly influenced by the work of people like Robert Trivers, Philip Tetlock, Hugo Mercier, Dan Sperber, Nichola Raihani, Robin Hanson, Tanya Luhrmann, Eric Funkhouser, and many more).
These are some of the topics that I will be writing about this year. I also have many interests, however, most of which I either research or lecture on, including:
Artificial intelligence
Mental illness and psychiatry
Politics
Neuroscience
Misinformation
Next week’s post…
Misinformation will be the topic of next week’s post, titled ‘There can’t be a science of misleading content’:
Brilliant stuff! I nodded along with a lot of what you said, but I'm afraid I disagree with you about the core of your argument, which I take to be: "To understand such evolutionarily weird behaviours, we should embrace Veblen’s core insight about luxury goods and services: human behaviours that seem odd, irrational, and wasteful are often rooted in social goals."
I have a bunch of disagreements, probably because I find more to agree with in Bataille's writing on waste and excess than Veblen or Mauss:
- All behaviour is weird and wasteful. This is because life is weird and wasteful. Waste does not serve a social function because waste is an end in itself (and, ultimately, what we desire - and the reason we accumulate power in the first place - is to escape and exceed systems of utilitarian logic and the regulation of waste). To say we waste in order to accumulate power doesn't tell me anything if the reason we accumulate that power is in order to waste.
- To prevent or reduce waste is to invite catastrophe, not for functionalist reasons, but rather because excess must be wasted. If excess energy is not wasted through ritualised / formalised sacrifice then it will be wasted through war or violence, and I worry that a functionalist approach to waste carries implicit normative claims aiming to make waste somehow more "efficient". I do see a distinction between catastrophic waste and productive waste, so perhaps this is somewhere our mental models overlap.
- More broadly, I worry that evolutionary explanations for social behaviour are deterministic, simplistic and (ultimately) politically reductive. I really enjoyed David Graeber and David Wengrow's discussion on potlach and waste in "The Dawn of Everything", and I thought they made important points criticising social evolution and emphasising multiplicity, change, and variety within cultures around the world historically.
Still, I hope you'll accept my comment not as criticism, but rather as an attempt to better understand your argument and test it against other ideas. I've subscribed, and I very much look forward to reading more!
Interesting essay - although not 100% clear that your somewhat contradicting ideas support your title thesis which is fine because as you write - humans are weird (complex) as compared to most other animals. And as many secular social science academics do - you seem to often mention/refer to evolution and its main founder, Darwin. Which I assume means you may be an atheist? I ask this because it pertains to my own ideas about prestige and power. As a spiritual person, my main concern about who to please or seek approval from is God first and then my wife and family friends etc. A prioritized social karass of ringlets/entities moving outward from me (like a pebble being dropped in a calm lake) to the rest of the universe I temporarily inhabit in this physical body. How do I know what pleases God - you are thinking? Like many others, I use my own mix of learned knowledge/wisdom of religions and philosophies from around the world (most overlap as far as good behavior concepts) and my own intuitive intelligence to guide me. Am I 100% sure I am right all the time - not at all - but when one is calm, mindful and satiated, one's inner moral compass is a trustworthy guide - our direct connection to the greater source. I was always popular throughout my life - probably because I am decent looking in a superficial world dominated by our eyes and not snobby based on my own big, typically American dysfunctional middle class family environment and moving around a lot during my childhood which forces one to either be extroverted or more introverted to navigate through our first collective tribal encounters with strangers aka schools. I chose extro (or it chose me?) - again based on my family/society environmental influences. Being an artist and as far as my own creative pursuits throughout my life - my own motives for doing so changed and evolved as I grew older - thank God! When I was young - my ego was huge and it was all about being a Rock Star which I pursued seriously for 15 years in LA and NYC - the core motives were carnal - getting hot women to hang out (wink-wink), which I guess would be a social aspect and of course getting rich enough to have FU money aka freedom from the stress caused from the lack thereof. The fame aspect would fit in with what you say about prestige - though I never achieved that, I did have some level of success but honestly having a big crowd dig your art is great but does not fill the hole in your soul - nor does sleeping with lots of women, unhealthy addictions to drugs, gambling and too much food etc. nor does money but I have to admit - to this day I still wish I had enough to be free and really pursue my art dreams full time - a luxury for folks lucky enough to have those resources available. And honestly I would not be heart broken if I never really make it as an artist while I am alive because some my heroes did not either. But I would love to win the lottery and live off the grid as much as is now possible - this my point - that goes against your over arching general social status hypothesis - because recognition and social status is not as important to me now as a middle aged man as is the freedom and luxury of just making art in a decent environment and being a good partner to my wife and son/brother to my family. But like most I have a day job and I like it most of the time. I am a middle school art teacher and it pays the bills and I do get some free time per year to do some art and writing etc. but the muse does not always line up with summer break! Motivation is the great mystery in education - how do I motivate my students to care about/respect Art and its unique role in our human development? Every day is a challenge and I dig it for the most part. Wow - what a tangent! Look forward to reading your future posts and some of the new school academics you mentioned. God bless and Happy New Year!